Psychophysics Behavior of Human Tactile Mechanism to Discriminate Extremely Thin Copper Foils.
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.14419/ijet.v7i4.27.22496Published:
2018-11-30Keywords:
Human tactile mechanism, Cutaneous function, Mechanoreceptor, Psychophysics, Weber fraction, Difference Threshold (DL).Abstract
In this study, our objective is to analyze human tactile mechanism behavior in recognizing extremely thin foils using the psychophysics method. Seven pairs of copper foils ranging in thickness from 20 ~ 150 μm were used in the experiment. We applied the method of constant stimuli to define the difference threshold between the materials. In order to increase the detection rate, contact between human tactile function and the metal foils was maintained. As a result, the Weber fraction c consistently reduces as thickness increases. However, the slope decrement is steep compared to previous experimentation (using stainless steel as base). We also validated the behavior of the undetected regions up to 150-μm thicknesses. This achievement was quite significant considering the higher thickness test ratio. In future, further comparison shall be made using different material type.
References
[1] Miyaoka, T. and Ohka, M. 2001. Tactile Information Processing Mechanisms to Discriminate Foil Thickness. Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics, Vol. 17.
[2] Miyaoka, T. and Ohka, M. 2002. The Duplex Theory of Thickness Discriminations by Touch. JSME Annual Meeting 2002(1), 231-232.
[3] Jones, L. A. and Lederman, S. J. 2006. Human Hand Function, Oxford University Press.
[4] Bossomaier, T. R. 2012. Introduction to the Senses: From Biology to Computer Science, Cambridge University Press.
[5] Mohammad Azzeim bin Mat Jusoh, M., Ohka, M., and Miyaoka, T. 2015. Finite Element Analysis of Human Tactile Sensing to Differentiate Thin Foils through Comparison Between Vertical & Angled Loads. Procedia Computer Science, Vol. 76: 40-46.
[6] Johansson, R.S. and Vallbo, A.B., 1979. Tactile Sensibility in the human hand: Relative and Absolute densities of four types of Mechanoreceptive Units in Glabrous Skin. J. Physiol., Vol. 286, 283-300.
[7] Johansson, R.S. and Flanagan, J.R., 2009. Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in object manipulation tasks. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(5), pp.345-359.
[8] Kandel, E.R., Schwartz, J.H., & Jessell, T.M. (Eds), 2000. Principles of Neural Science (Fifth Edition, pp. 500). New York: McGraw-Hill.
[9] Johanness, Z. 2010. Sensation, Perception and Action. An Evolutionary Perspective. Palgrave Macmillian.
[10] Blausen.com staff. Blausen Gallery 2014. Wikiversity Journal of Medicine. DOI:10.15347/wjm/2014.010.ISSN 20018762
[11] Gescheider G. A. 1997. Psychophysics: The Fundamentals, Third ed., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[12] Manning, S. A. and Rosenstock, E. H. 1968. Classical Psychophysics and Scaling, McGraw-Hill Book Company.
[13] Stevens, S. S. 1975. Psychophysics: Introduction to Its Perceptual, Neural and Social Prospects, John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
[14] John, K. T., Goodwin, A. W., and Darian-Smith, I. 1989. Tactile Discrimination of Thickness. Experimental Brain Research, Vol. 78, No. 1: 62-68.
[15] Jusoh, Mohammad Azzeim Mat, Masahiro Ohka, and Tetsu Miyaoka. "Comparison of Tactile Discriminations to Verify the Undetectable Region of SUS Foil Thickness." Journal of Telecommunication, Electronic and Computer Engineering (JTEC) 8, no. 7 (2016): 83-86.
[16] Sugiman, Kenji, Masahiro Ohka, and Mohammad Azzeim bin Mat Jusoh. "A Basic Paper Handling Task Experiment Using Tri-axial Tactile Data." Procedia Computer Science 105 (2017): 270-275.
[17] Read, J.C.A. 2015. The place of Human Psychophysics in modern Neuroscience. Neuroscience, 296, pp. 116-129.
[18] Fujita, K. and Ohmori, H. 2001. A New Softness Display Interface by Dynamic Fingertip Contact Area Control. In 5th World Multiconference on Systemics, Cybernetics and Informatics, pp. 78-82.
[19] Burgess, P.R. and Perl, E.R., 1973. Cutaneous Mechanoreceptors and Nocireceptors. Somatosensory System, Springer.
[20] Srinivasan, M.A. and LaMotte, R.H. 1987. Tactile Discrimination of Shape: Response of Slowly and Rapidly Adapting Mechanoreceptive Afferrents to a step indented into the monkey fingerpad. The Journal of Neuroscience, 7(6), 1682-1697.
[21] Goodwin, A.W., Macefield, V.G., & Bisley, J.W. 1997. Encoding of Object Curviture by Tactile Afferrents from Human Fingers. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78(6), 2881-2888.
[22] Bolanowski, S.J. Jr., Gescheider, G.A., Verrillo, R.T. and Checkosky, C.M. 1988. Four Channels Mediate the Mechanical Aspects of Touch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 84, 1680-1694.
How to Cite
License
Authors who publish with this journal agree to the following terms:- Authors retain copyright and grant the journal right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution Licensethat allows others to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work''s authorship and initial publication in this journal.
- Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the journal''s published version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgement of its initial publication in this journal.
- Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work (See The Effect of Open Access).
Accepted 2018-11-30
Published 2018-11-30